- Research article
- Open access
- Published:
New words in human mutagenesis
BMC Bioinformatics volume 12, Article number: 268 (2011)
Abstract
Background
The substitution rates within different nucleotide contexts are subject to varying levels of bias. The most well known example of such bias is the excess of C to T (C > T) mutations in CpG (CG) dinucleotides. The molecular mechanisms underlying this bias are important factors in human genome evolution and cancer development. The discovery of other nucleotide contexts that have profound effects on substitution rates can improve our understanding of how mutations are acquired, and why mutation hotspots exist.
Results
We compared rates of inherited mutations in 1-4 bp nucleotide contexts using reconstructed ancestral states of human single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) from intergenic regions. Chimp and orangutan genomic sequences were used as outgroups. We uncovered 3.5 and 3.3-fold excesses of T > C mutations in the second position of ATTG and ATAG words, respectively, and a 3.4-fold excess of A > C mutations in the first position of the ACAA word.
Conclusions
Although all the observed biases are less pronounced than the 5.1-fold excess of C > T mutations in CG dinucleotides, the three 4 bp mutation contexts mentioned above (and their complementary contexts) are well distinguished from all other mutation contexts. This provides a challenge to discover the underlying mechanisms responsible for the observed excesses of mutations.
Background
A cytosine followed by a guanine (CG) is the best known example of a nucleotide word within the human genome that has a dramatically increased probability to undergo mutation [1]. By the early 1960s, researchers already knew that many animal genomes have a deficit in CG dinucleotides [2, 3]. This was later explained by DNA methylation. Specific DNA methyltransferase enzymes convert cytosines in CG dinucleotides into methyl-cytosines, which are susceptible to deamination to thymine [4]. This mutation mechanism is involved in cancer development [5], has shaped the composition of our genome [6, 7], and remains profoundly interesting to molecular and evolutionary biologists today [8].
Other sequences within the genome are subject to biases of varying magnitude [9–12], and knowledge of these biases has contributed greatly to our understanding of the molecular mechanisms involved in mutagenesis [13, 14]. A nucleotide context affects not only the rate of substitution, but also the rates of deletions and insertions [15]. Mutation rates within sequences are also influenced by local CpG content [16], are dependent on the chromosome on which the sequences is located [17, 18], and vary between different regions on the same chromosome [19, 20]. Additionally, excessive amounts of mutations have been reported in certain repeated elements [21]. The combination of different mutation factors and selection has led to the existence of mutation hot-spots [10].
With the exception of the CG sequence, relationships between substitution rates and neighbouring nucleotides in the human germline are still poorly understood. To the best of our knowledge, no other sequences have been shown to influence the rates of inheritable mutations in a way comparable to the effect of CG dinucleotides on C > T transition rates. The goal of the present study was to identify such sequences. For such a study, the ideal would be to analyze the genome sequences of parents and their children as a direct source of mutation data. However, such data are currently unavailable. Therefore, we used a dataset of presumably neutral mutations that occurred in the human lineage after the separation of humans and chimpanzees. Such data can be derived from human intergenic SNPs for which the ancestral state has been reconstructed.
We show that the mutation rates within three 4 bp nucleotide contexts in the human genome (and their complementary contexts) stand out from the mutation rates of all other 2-4 bp mutation contexts. The effects of these contexts on mutation rates is comparable to the effect of CG nucleotides on C > T mutation rates.
Results
To study mutation processes separately (as far as possible) from the effects of natural selection, we used human single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) from regions that do not belong to any known genes or CpG islands, and are not within 1000 bp of flanking regions of known genes. A genomic polymorphism in the human population can be attributed, in the vast majority of cases, to a relatively recent mutation. Theoretically, a polymorphism may be inherited from a common ancestor of human and apes, but the proportion of such polymorphisms seems to be relatively small [10]. Indeed, most of them should be rare cases of stabilizing selection in favour of the polymorphism. Thus, it is possible to compare each human SNP with the corresponding nucleotides in the genomic sequences of apes, namely chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus). In cases where both ape nucleotides coincide with one of the human SNP variants, we can identify the ancestral state of the SNP, i.e., the direction of the mutation. Using this method, we identified the direction of 3,405,095 probable mutations. This mutation data are available to download at: http://mouse.belozersky.msu.ru/SNP/. UCSC Human Genome Browser mapping of human SNPs to chimp and orangutan genomes was used [22]. This amount of data was judged to be sufficient for the analysis of 1-4 bp mutation contexts.
We used a measure called 'contrast' to evaluate if the addition of specific nucleotides to the 5' or 3' end of 1-3 bp words increases the probability of observing certain mutations in fixed positions (see Methods). For example, there is a 5.1-fold excess of C to T (C > T) mutations if C is followed by G, when compared with the rate of C > T mutations on average in the genome. We say that the mutation context description is {C > T|1, CG}, and that it has a contrast of 5.1 when compared with its {C > T|1, C} subcontext. Contrast values higher than 1 represent an excess of mutations, while values smaller than 1 represent mutation deficiency. Contrast values for a mutation context {mut|pos, W} and subcontext {mut|pos', W'} are computed based on the occurrences of words W and W' and the number of mutations observed in {mut|pos, W} and {mut|pos', W'}, respectively (see Methods). To avoid misunderstanding, note that our concept of contrast is not related to the contrast between the number of samples used in variance analysis.
Each mutation context can be characterized by two contrast values: mutation bias and minimal contrast. The minimal contrast value of a context is its contrast value closest to 1 among all contrast values, when compared with each of the contexts subcontexts. For example, the {C > T|2, ACG} context has three such subcontexts {C > T|2, AC}, {C > T|1, CG}, and {C > T|1, C} giving contrast values of 5.08, 1.08 and 5.48, respectively. 1.08 is the minimal contrast for {C > T|2, ACG}. Contrast values obtained using one-letter subcontexts such as {C > T|1, C} are called mutation biases. The value 5.48 is the mutation bias for {C > T|2, ACG} because there is a 5.48-fold excess of C > T mutations in position two of the word ACG, when compared with the average C > T mutation rate in the genome.
Mutation bias indicates the total excess (or deficiency) of mutations within a given context. Minimal contrast indicates the excess (or deficiency) of mutations within a given context that cannot be explained by the excess (or deficiency) of mutations in one of its subcontexts, thus representing the actual role of the context as a whole. For dinucleotide contexts such as {C > T|1, CG}, mutation bias equals minimal contrast.
Figure 1 contains a two-dimensional plot of mutation bias versus minimal contrast; each dot represents a mutation context. The current analysis does not allow us to discern the strand of DNA on which a mutation occurred. This is why each mutation context has a complementary context with similar mutation properties: dots are situated on the plot in pairs. On the plot, besides the large cluster that includes the majority of contexts, we can see three distinct small clusters. As would be expected, the cluster characterized by the highest value of minimal contrast consists of two dots, representing the {C > T|1, CG} context and its complementary {G > A|2, CG} context. A second cluster is characterized by low minimal contrast and high mutation bias, and consists of all contexts, and only such contexts, that have {C > T|1, CG} or {G > A|2, CG} subcontexts. Finally, there is a distinct cluster that is characterized by both high mutation bias and minimal contrast. It contains three pairs of mutation contexts. These contexts are {T > C|2, ATTG} and its complement {A > G|3, CAAT}; {T > C|2, ATAG} and {A > G|3, CTAT}; and {A > C|1, ACAA} and {T > G|4, TTGT}.
On Figure 2, the distributions of mutation bias and minimal contrast values for all 1-4 bp contexts are shown. The enlarged fragment (Figure 2B) demonstrates that the contexts {T > C|2, ATTG} and {A > G|3, CAAT}, {T > C|2, ATAG} and {A > G|3, CTAT}, {A > C|1, ACAA} and {T > G|4, TTGT}, along with {C > T|1, CG}- and {G > A|2, CG}-containing contexts are well distinguished by their mutation biases. Additionally, it can be noted that all 5 bp mutation contexts containing {T > C|2, ATTG}, {A > G|3, CAAT}, {T > C|2, ATAG}, {A > G|3, CTAT}, {A > C|1, ACAA}, and {T > G|4, TTGT} subcontexts also have high mutation bias values (data not shown). The eight contexts{C > T|1, CG} and {G > A|2, CG}; {T > C|2, ATTG} and {A > G|3, CAAT}; {T > C|2, ATAG} and {A > G|3, CTAT}; {A > C|1, ACAA} and {T > G|4, TTGT} also have the highest minimal contrast values (Figure 2D).
It is difficult to predict the distribution of mutation biases and minimal contrasts theoretically, because of the dependence of contexts: one mutation from the data set is considered in all possible contexts of 1-4 bp length, which are clearly not independent. Indeed, it is unclear how to approximate the distribution of mutation biases (Figure 2A). Surprisingly, in the range 0.7 - 1.2 the distribution of minimal contrast values can be approximated by the normal distribution with the mean of 0.99 and a sigma value of 0.12 (Figure 2C). In the range 1.2 - 5.1 these distributions are significantly different at p < 0.001 (Figure 2D). If we assume that mutations are random and that the normal distribution reflects the distribution of minimal contrast values, then over one hundred 1-4 bp contexts may significantly influence mutation rates. The eight mentioned contexts are the most extreme among them.
There are 3.5 and 3.3-fold excesses of T > C mutations in position two of ATTG and ATAG words, respectively, suggesting that a highly mutable pattern AT[A/T]G (ATWG) exists in the human genome (note that complementary contexts behave in the same way). There is also a 3.4-fold excess of A > C mutations in position one of the word ACAA. These effects are comparable to the 5.1-fold excess of C > T mutations in the first position of the CG dinucleotide. The existence of excessive mutations in each of the described contexts compared with any of their subcontexts is statistically significant at p < 10-15, taking the Bonferroni correction into account. Our main results did not change when we used SNPs from the whole human genome (6,530,908 SNPs) (Table 1). We also found 2890 (out of 3744) other contexts with mutation rates significantly different at p < 10-15 from the mutation rates of each of their subcontexts. However, here we would like to concentrate on the most extreme cases, which are comparable with {C > T|1, CG} in terms of contrast values and mutation biases.
Discussion
In our study, we used parsimony to infer the ancestral states of the SNPs. However, there is a risk of misinterpretation when using parsimony and dealing with rapid mutations. For example, it has been stressed that if the human sequence is CG, the chimp sequence is TG, and a TG sequence from the baboon is used as an outgroup, the human and chimp ancestral sequence is actually more likely to be CG, not TG as parsimony would predict [23].This occurs because of highly elevated CG > TG mutation rates. However, in our study, instead of just one outgroup species, two outgroup species were used, and the ancestral state of two human SNP variants was inferred by comparing those variants with chimp and orangutan sequences. Any scenario alternative to the one estimated by parsimony would require at least three alternative mutations, not just two.
To reduce the effects of natural selection we analyzed only those DNA regions that are far away from the known genes. However, several studies have shown that selective constraints can be found in non-coding and even in intergenic regions of the human genome [24, 25]. Also, regions subject to accelerated human evolution, possibly caused by positive selection, were found to be enriched in gene deserts (regions > 500 kb without an Ensembl gene) [26]. Intergenic regions that were found to be subject to selective constraints or accelerated evolution were not specifically excluded from our analysis. We postulated that the total length of such regions would be too small to have a pronounced effect on the observed genome-wide mutation rates.
Substitution rate biases were recently studied by Nevarez et al. using a measure called relative abundance [27]. Relative abundance indicates whether mutations in a context happen at rates different from those expected from the mutation frequencies of all of that context's subcontexts, including discontigous ones (for example, ATNG). Although none of the mutation contexts that we have identified using the minimal contrast method were highlighted in that study as being exceptional, it is worth mentioning that the {T > C|2, ATTG} context has the highest relative abundance among all 4-bp mutation contexts. The {T > C|2, ATNG} discontigous context (a less specific representation of the {T > C|2, ATWG} motif we identified) also had high relative abundance. However, the {A > C|1, ACAA} context did not. Also, it is important to note that in the study by Nevarez et al, substitutions were studied, not SNPs. It can be argued that fixed substitutions should be, on average, under higher selective pressure than SNPs. SNPs are not fixed in the population, and are likely to be more neutral.
Before comparing relative abundance and minimal contrast, the two values used for measuring the influence of contexts on mutation rates, it should be noted that, at the moment, there is no universal statistical model that can adequately describe the frequencies of short nucleotide words in a genome. The lack of such a model can be demonstrated using the example of 3 bp word frequencies in the human genome. Previously, we found that all observed frequencies of 3 bp words significantly differ (p < 10-6) from the expected frequencies based on the frequencies of single nucleotides and 2 bp words, even with the best models among the many different models found in the literature [7].
The lack of a universally accepted statistical model of the genome has led to authors using different approximations that are most suitable for their research task. For a genome-scale comparison of word frequencies, we believe the Relative Abundance Value, suggested by Karlin et al. [28] to be the most balanced approximation. This value was used in a number of subsequent studies, including one of our own [7]. Relative abundance was modified to estimate the frequencies of mutations in nucleotide contexts in the article by Nevarez et al. [27]. These values should give a good approximation of the expected mutation frequencies, and their comparison with the observed frequencies should reveal the complete picture of a context's impact on mutation frequencies.
In the present study, we used different values - minimal contrast and mutation bias - because our goal was to find those contexts, besides {C > T|1,CG}), that have a dramatic effect on mutation rates. If context {mut|pos, w} is highly mutable because of the existence of some context-dependent mutation mechanism, then the number of mutations observed in this context will be significantly higher than expected from the mutation rates in no particular context (mutation bias) or in any of its subcontexts (minimal contrast). The distribution of mutation rates among subcontexts is not important in this case.
To produce a complete picture of the context's dependence of mutation frequencies, minimal contrast is, of course, not as good as relative abundance. For example, in cases when a context's contrast with one subcontext is -0.91 and for the other it is +1.09, the minimal contrast will be smaller or larger than 1 depending on the number in the third decimal position of the contrast values. This value is obviously unstable. However, this is not the case for any of the contexts that are highlighted in our article. Despite this problem, minimal context allowed us to reach our goal and identify several contexts that dramatically affect mutation rates.
As previously mentioned, the elevated frequency of mutations in the {C > T|1,CG} context in the human genome is consistent with the underrepresentation of CpG dinucleotides. It would not be surprising if the words ATTG, ATAG, and ACAA, which are present in the highly mutable contexts described in our study, would also be underrepresented in the human genome, and in the genomes of closely related species. Data on the underrepresentation and overrepresentation of 1-7 bp words in 139 complete genomes were recently published [7]. While ATTG seems to be slightly underrepresented in all 22 studied mammalian genomes (including the human genome), ATAG is underrepresented in only 13 mammalian genomes, and ACAA is actually slightly overrepresented in all but two mammalian genomes. This can be explained either by the novelty of the underlying mechanisms that lead to excessive mutations in the {T > C|2,ATTG}, {T > C|2,ATAG}, and {A > C|1,ACAA} contexts or by the fact that other mutations leading to the loss or accumulation of ATAG, ATTG, and ACAA words are not accounted for. It is also worth mentioning that none of the three mentioned mutation contexts have elevated minimal contrast or mutation bias values in Drosophila melanogaster (unpublished data).
Excessive mutations in the {T > C|2,ATTG}, {T > C|2,ATAG}, and {A > C|1,ACAA} contexts suggest the possible existence of underlying mechanisms in a similar way in which DNA methyltransferase activity is at least partially responsible for excessive mutations in the {C > T|1,CG} context.
Conclusions
Three 4 bp mutation contexts with contrastingly high mutation rates exist in the human genome, suggesting the existence of previously unknown context-dependent molecular mechanisms involved in human mutagenesis and providing challenges for further experimental research. Two of these contexts can be combined into one highly mutable motif (AT[A/T]G). The excess of mutations in these contexts is not explained by excessive mutations in their subcontexts.
Methods
SNP inclusion criteria
Human SNPs (dbSNP 130) from regions aligned to chimpanzee and orangutan genomes, according to the UCSC Human Genome Browser [12], where obtained. To reduce bias from natural selection we excluded SNPs from any UCSC genes, CpG islands, or flanking regions within 1000 bp of UCSC genes. Coordinates of CpG islands were also taken from the UCSC Human Genome Browser. SNPs were included only if the following prerequisites were met:
-
(1)
Exactly two human SNP variants are reported in the SNP database.
-
(2)
One of the two human SNP variants is the same as the orthologous nucleotides in both chimp and orangutan genomes.
-
(3)
10 bp upstream and downstream regions adjacent to the human SNP (SNP regions) are aligned to the chimpanzee and orangutan genomes, as reported by the UCSC database, and the corresponding sequences (orthologous regions) in these genomes can be identified.
-
(4)
The SNP regions and the corresponding orthologous regions in chimpanzee and orangutan genomes do not contain gaps or unknown nucleotides.
-
(5)
The SNP regions differ by no more than by one substitution compared with the orthologous regions in chimpanzee (the SNP position is not taken into account).
-
(6)
The SNP regions differ by no more than six substitutions compared with the orthologous regions in orangutan.
-
(7)
Three nucleotides upstream, as well as three nucleotides downstream, of the SNP are the same in human and chimp genomes.
Mutation context and subcontext
We denote the mutation context of mutation mut in position pos of the word W as {mut|pos, W}. For example, {C > T|1, CG} represents a C > T mutation in the first position of the word CG.
Mutation context {mut|pos', W'} is called a subcontext of the context {mut|pos, W} if W' is a subword of W and any mutation mut occurring in position pos of the word W is at the same time a mutation occurring in position pos' of the word W'. For example, {C > T|1, CG} is a subcontext of {C > T|2, ACG}.
Contrast
For each pair of context {mut|pos, W} and its subcontext {mut|pos', W'} the value of contrast is given by the formula:
Here P{mut|pos, W} and P{mut|pos', W'} are the conditional probabilities of observing mutation mut in the position pos of the word W, and position pos' of word W', respectively, in a given dataset. Although these probabilities cannot be explicitly calculated without assumptions of the general probability of mutation per nucleotide in the genome, their ratio can be estimated by the following formula:
Here, P W and P W' are the observed frequencies of words W and W' respectively among all words of the same length in the following intervals adjacent to the human SNP: from position -10 to position -6 and from position 6 to position 10 inclusively. We also used word frequencies from the entire human genome as a separate control. N{mut|pos, W} and N{mut|pos', W'} are the numbers of observed mutations in the {mut|pos, W} context and {mut|pos', W'} subcontext, respectively.
The ratio P W /P W' estimates the probability for W' to be extended to W. This ratio coincides with the expected ratio N{mut|pos, W}/N{mut|pos', W'} under the hypothesis that mutations rates are the same in the context {mut|pos, W} and its subcontext {mut|pos', W'}. Therefore, if Contrast ({mut|pos, W}, {mut|pos', W'}) is greater than 1, it indicates an increased mutation rate in the context {mut|pos, W} compared with the subcontext {mut|pos', W'}; while if Contrast ({mut|pos, W}, {mut|pos', W'}) is less than 1, it indicates a decreased mutation rate.
Minimal contrast
For a given context {mut|pos, W} let us consider all of its subcontexts {mut|pos', W'}. The minimal contrast is the value MC = Contrast ({mut|pos, W}, {mut|pos', W'}) such that the absolute difference |MC - 1| is the lowest among all subcontexts {mut|pos', W'}. We did not study discontigous contexts.
Mutation bias
For any context {mut|pos, W} there exist only one subcontext {mut|pos', W'} such that the length of W' is equal to 1 (i.e., W' is the one-letter word, consisting of the mutated letter). The mutation bias is the contrast of the given context and this subcontext.
Statistical significance
The statistical significance of the hypothesis that there is an excess of mutation mut in the context {mut|pos, W} compared with its subcontext {mut|pos', W'} can be computed using a binomial distribution with the following parameters:
The significance level was set to 10-15 taking into account the Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. There were a total of 13728 multiple comparisons of context and subcontext pairs.
References
Bird AP: DNA methylation and the frequency of CpG in animal DNA. Nucleic Acids Res 1980, 8(7):1499–1504. 10.1093/nar/8.7.1499
Josse J, Kaiser AD, Kornberg A: Enzymatic synthesis of deoxyribonucleic acid. VIII. Frequencies of nearest neighbor base sequences in deoxyribonucleic acid. J Biol Chem 1961, 236: 864–875.
Swartz MN, Trautner TA, Kornberg A: Enzymatic synthesis of deoxyribonucleic acid. XI. Further studies on nearest neighbor base sequences in deoxyribonucleic acids. J Biol Chem 1962, 237: 1961–1967.
Cooper DN, Krawczak M: Cytosine methylation and the fate of CpG dinucleotides in vertebrate genomes. Hum Genet 1989, 83(2):181–188. 10.1007/BF00286715
Kanai Y, Hirohashi S: Alterations of DNA methylation associated with abnormalities of DNA methyltransferases in human cancers during transition from a precancerous to a malignant state. Carcinogenesis 2007, 28(12):2434–2442. 10.1093/carcin/bgm206
Burge C, Campbell AM, Karlin S: Over- and under-representation of short oligonucleotides in DNA sequences. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1992, 89(4):1358–1362. 10.1073/pnas.89.4.1358
Mitrofanov SI, Panchin AY, Spirin SA, Alexeevski AV, Panchin YV: Exclusive sequences of different genomes. J Bioinform Comput Biol 2010, 8(3):519–534. 10.1142/S0219720010004719
Zemach A, McDaniel IE, Silva P, Zilberman D: Genome-wide evolutionary analysis of eukaryotic DNA methylation. Science 2010, 328(5980):916–919. 10.1126/science.1186366
Arnheim N, Calabrese P: Understanding what determines the frequency and pattern of human germline mutations. Nat Rev Genet 2009, 10(7):478–488.
Hodgkinson A, Ladoukakis E, Eyre-Walker A: Cryptic variation in the human mutation rate. PLoS Biol 2009, 7(2):e1000027.
Blake RD, Hess ST, Nicholson-Tuell J: The influence of nearest neighbors on the rate and pattern of spontaneous point mutations. J Mol Evol 1992, 34(3):189–200. 10.1007/BF00162968
Hwang DG, Green P: Bayesian Markov chain Monte Carlo sequence analysis reveals varying neutral substitution patterns in mammalian evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2004, 101(39):13994–14001. 10.1073/pnas.0404142101
Rogozin IB, Pavlov YI: Theoretical analysis of mutation hotspots and their DNA sequence context specificity. Mutat Res 2003, 544(1):65–85. 10.1016/S1383-5742(03)00032-2
Rogozin IB, Malyarchuk BA, Pavlov YI, Milanesi L: From context-dependence of mutations to molecular mechanisms of mutagenesis. Pac Symp Biocomput 2005, 409–420.
Kondrashov AS, Rogozin IB: Context of deletions and insertions in human coding sequences. Hum Mutat 2004, 23(2):177–185. 10.1002/humu.10312
Walser JC, Furano AV: The mutational spectrum of non-CpG DNA varies with CpG content. Genome Res 2010, 20(7):875–882. 10.1101/gr.103283.109
Miyata T, Hayashida H, Kuma K, Mitsuyasu K, Yasunaga T: Male-driven molecular evolution: a model and nucleotide sequence analysis. Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol 1987, 52: 863–867.
Makova KD, Li WH: Strong male-driven evolution of DNA sequences in humans and apes. Nature 2002, 416(6881):624–626. 10.1038/416624a
Gaffney DJ, Keightley PD: The scale of mutational variation in the murid genome. Genome Res 2005, 15(8):1086–1094. 10.1101/gr.3895005
Matassi G, Sharp PM, Gautier C: Chromosomal location effects on gene sequence evolution in mammals. Curr Biol 1999, 9(15):786–791. 10.1016/S0960-9822(99)80361-3
Jeffreys AJ, Royle NJ, Wilson V, Wong Z: Spontaneous mutation rates to new length alleles at tandem-repetitive hypervariable loci in human DNA. Nature 1988, 332(6161):278–281. 10.1038/332278a0
Rhead B, Karolchik D, Kuhn RM, Hinrichs AS, Zweig AS, Fujita PA, Diekhans M, Smith KE, Rosenbloom KR, Raney BJ, et al.: The UCSC Genome Browser database: update 2010. Nucleic Acids Res 2010, 38(Database issue):D613–619.
Duret L: The GC content of primates and rodents genomes is not at equilibrium: a reply to Antezana. J Mol Evol 2006, 62(6):803–806. 10.1007/s00239-005-0228-7
Bush EC, Lahn BT: Selective constraint on noncoding regions of hominid genomes. PLoS Comput Biol 2005, 1(7):e73. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0010073
Shabalina SA, Ogurtsov AY, Kondrashov VA, Kondrashov AS: Selective constraint in intergenic regions of human and mouse genomes. Trends Genet 2001, 17(7):373–376. 10.1016/S0168-9525(01)02344-7
Bush EC, Lahn BT: A genome-wide screen for noncoding elements important in primate evolution. BMC Evol Biol 2008, 8: 17. 10.1186/1471-2148-8-17
Nevarez PA, DeBoever CM, Freeland BJ, Quitt MA, Bush EC: Context dependent substitution biases vary within the human genome. BMC Bioinformatics 2010, 11: 462. 10.1186/1471-2105-11-462
Karlin S, Cardon LR: Computational DNA sequence analysis. Annu Rev Microbiol 1994, 48: 619–654. 10.1146/annurev.mi.48.100194.003155
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to Georgii A Bazykin and Alexander I Tuzhikov for helping us improve the manuscript. This work was supported by RFBR grants 11-04-01511, 08-04-00478, 11-04-91340, and 09-04-92743, MCB RAS, and state contracts P810 and 14.740.11.1202 of the Federal Special Program "Scientific and educational human resources of innovative Russia" for 2009 - 2013.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Authors' contributions
AP, SM, AA, SS, and YP have participated equally in the design of the study, data preparation, and drafting the manuscript. AA performed the statistical analysis. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Authors’ original submitted files for images
Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.
Rights and permissions
This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
About this article
Cite this article
Panchin, A.Y., Mitrofanov, S.I., Alexeevski, A.V. et al. New words in human mutagenesis. BMC Bioinformatics 12, 268 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2105-12-268
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2105-12-268